Which of the following help us estimate objects’ size and distance to aid in our perception?

  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts
  • PMC2728930

Exp Brain Res. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 Apr 1.

Show

Published in final edited form as:

PMCID: PMC2728930

NIHMSID: NIHMS129426

Abstract

Accurate distance perception depends on the processing and integration of a variety of monocular and binocular cues. Dorsal stream lesions can impair this process, but details of this neurocognitive relationship remain unclear. Here, we tested a patient with bilateral occipitoparietal damage and severely impaired stereopsis. We addressed four related questions: 1) Can distance and size perception survive limitations in perceiving monocular and binocular cues? 2) Are egocentric (self-referential) and allocentric (object-referential) distance judgments similarly impaired? 3) Are distance measurements equally impaired in peripersonal and extrapersonal space? 4) Are size judgments possible when distance processing is impaired? The results demonstrate that the patient's lesions impaired both her distance and size perception, but not uniformly. Her performance when using an egocentric reference frame was more impaired than her performance when using an allocentric reference frame. Likewise, her distance judgments in peripersonal space were more impaired than those in extrapersonal space. The patient showed partial preservation in size processing of novel objects even when familiar size cues were removed.

Keywords: distance, depth, parietal, stereopsis, simultanagnosia, Balint's syndrome

Impaired Distance Perception and Size Constancy Following Bilateral Occipitoparietal Damage

A veridical representation of the three-dimensional world requires accurate perception of object distance and object size. Distance perception requires the integration of diverse cues; size perception is determined by size constancy, the scaling of an object's retinal subtense by estimated distance (Emmert, 1881). The neural correlates of these processes have long been linked to dorsal stream function. Ninety years ago, Holmes and Horrax reported the case study of a soldier with bilateral parietal lobe lesions who exhibited extensive distance perception deficits (Holmes & Horrax, 1919). This patient could not determine the closer of two objects even though he possessed normal visual acuity. He had also lost stereopsis, a binocular distance cue. Additional reports have linked distance perception deficits with dorsal stream damage (Critchley, 1953; Holmes, 1918; Riddoch, 1917). Other reports have related dorsal stream damage to deficits in size constancy (Ferber & Danckert, 2006; Rode, Michel, Rossetti, Boisson, & Vallar, 2006; Wyke, 1960). In such classic neuropsychological reports lesion location is roughly estimated and deficits are based on bedside clinical evaluations.

More recently, a number of fMRI studies have investigated stereopsis and have identified a number of dorsal stream areas associated with stereopsis, such as retinotopic area V3A and caudal intraparietal sulcus (Backus, Fleet, Parker, and Heeger, 2001; Brouwer, van Ee, and Schwarzbach, 2005; Inui et al., 2000; Iwami et al., 2002; Kwee, Fujii, Matsuzawa, and Nakada, 1999; Naganuma et al., 2005; Neri, Bridge, and Heeger, 2004; Nishida et al., 2001; Rutschmann and Greenlee, 2004; Rutschmann, Schrauf, and Greenlee, 2000; Tsao et al., 2003; see review in Parker and Cumming, 2001). However, stereopsis is not the sole cue used to assess object distance. In fact, individuals who have lost the use of one eye and are restricted to monocular cues generally show little impairment in everyday situations (reviewed in Steeves, Gonzalez, and Steinbach, 2008).

In the present study, we investigated aspects of distance perception in a single patient with bilateral occipitoparietal damage. This patient suffers from many of the neurological symptoms associated with Balint's syndrome: simultanagnosia, optic ataxia, and distance perception deficits (Balint, 1909; Holmes and Horrax, 1919; see discussion in Harvey and Milner, 1995). Patient EE555 reports misjudging distances, bumping into objects, and accidentally poking people in the face. However, she reports no difficulty estimating object size. The following experiments were motivated by this apparent contradiction: inaccurate distance estimation accompanied by accurate size estimation, and also by the fact that newer research on the parietal lobes provides clues as to which variables that should modulate distance perception. In Part 1, we examined patient EE555's ability to access several common monocular distance cues. In Part 2, we investigated EE555's ability to estimate distance according to egocentric or allocentric reference frames. In Part 3 we investigated EE555's ability to estimate the size of novel objects.

General Methods

Patient EE555

Patient EE555 (female, age 39, 16 years education) experienced three parietal lobe infarcts in the watershed between the posterior and middle cerebral arteries between May-June 2004. MRI revealed symmetrical lesions in lateral inferior parietal regions, extending from the occipital lobe (superior middle occipital gyrus Brodmann areas (BA) 19) through the angular gyrus (BA 39) in and around inferior and middle portions of the IPS. Damage did not encroach into the precuneus or superior parietal lobe; see Figure 1a. EE555 was tested in several sessions at least 30 months after her last stroke and her condition remains stable.

Which of the following help us estimate objects’ size and distance to aid in our perception?

a) MRI brain images (T2 FLAIR) of Patient EE555. Hypodensities indicate lesion sites. b) Example occlusion stimuli. To patient EE555, the image on the left is a ‘daisy’, whereas the figure on the right is a ‘green oval’. Due to her simultanagnosia, the underlying yellow and blue ellipses in the right image are not perceived. c) Example stimuli used in Experiment 1. d) Example stimuli used in Experiment 2. e) Example of 2-D (left) and 3-D (right) stimuli used in Experiment 3.

Table 1 summarizes EE555's behavioral testing. She shows two of the three cardinal symptoms of Balint's syndrome: simultanagnosia and moderate optic ataxia. Simultanagnosia is illustrated by her inability to perceive scenes or to perceive more than one object simultaneously. For example, she demonstrates complete local bias with Navon letters. Optic ataxia is illustrated by her aberrant reaching behavior: she reaches beyond objects with either hand and trawls for them with an open hand.

Table 1

Summary of Patient EE555's perceptual and motoric symptoms. A “+” indicates normal performance, a “−” indicates abnormal performance.

TestPerformance
Color perception +
Object identification +
Sound identification +
Illusory contour perception (Kanisza) +
Optic ataxia: reach towards object
Optic apraxia +
Eye movements: horizontal +
Eye movements: rapid tracking
Left-right orientation +
Up-down orientation +
Finger identification
Scene perception: cookie theft
Local/global processing: Navon letters
Line Cancellation
Language: Western Aphasia Battery +

EE555 has normal visual acuity (20/20) and preserved color and shape perception as determined by ceiling performance on card-matching tasks (e.g. “Find the yellow square”). She has normal object recognition as shown by her ability to name common objects when shown line drawings or photographs. She can visually track slowly moving objects, and can maintain convergence on objects moving towards or away from her in the z-plane. It is difficult to evaluate EE555's visual fields because she cannot simultaneously fixate centrally and perceive peripheral objects.

We tested stereopsis using the TNO test for stereoscopic vision (Lameris Ootech, Nieuwegein, The Netherlands). EE555 was able to perceive only the first two pre-screening images. This finding indicates that her stereopsis is less than 240 seconds of arc at best.

All experimental protocols were approved by the Internal Review Board of the University of Pennsylvania and Temple University. All participants signed informed consent documents and were compensated for their time.

Part 1: Perception of Monocular Distance Cues

Patients with occipitoparietal damage can experience severely impaired distance perception (Holmes and Horrax, 1919). It is not clear to what extent this type of brain damage disrupts perception of monocular distance cues, those cues perceived by a single eye. Our preliminary testing established that stereopsis, a primary binocular cue, is severely impaired in patient EE555. In Part 1, we assessed patient EE555's ability to utilize certain monocular distance cues under optimal viewing conditions. Most monocular cues are pictorial in that they are intrinsic to the monocular image and are present in 2-dimensional renderings such as photographs. These include: interposition, familiar size, texture, shadow, linear perspective, and relative height. We conducted testing with a subset of these cues and excluded others because they are not relevant to processing near items (e.g. atmospheric haze), or because simultanagnosia made it impossible for patient EE555 to observe the cue (e.g. the separate lines used to create linear perspective).

Experiment 1: Occlusion

Occlusion is a dominant monocular depth cue. Our preliminary phenomenological observations indicated that EE555's simultanagnosia limited her access to occlusion cues. For example, EE555 described three identical overlapping ellipses as a ‘daisy’; see Figure 1b. In contrast, when the ellipses were different colors, EE555 perceived only the unoccluded ellipse. In order to investigate this further, a series of simple overlapping images were presented.

Ten images of two overlapping geometric shapes (circle, square, triangle, diamond) were constructed and presented using ePrime software on a Dell laptop computer. In each image there was one red and one blue object. The patient was told that there were exactly two objects in each display and was instructed to make a 2-alternative forced choice judgment indicating which object was in front: red or blue (chance = 50%). In this and all experiments, viewing time was unlimited, responses were unspeeded and normal lighting conditions were used. Stimuli were randomized and each stimulus was presented twice. There were 20 trials.

EE555 provided the correct answer on 75% (15/20) of the trials. These findings show that EE555 has reduced access to occlusion cues.

Experiment 2: Perspective in 2D and 3D Shapes

This experiment assessed Patient EE555's ability to integrate linear perspective cues. The task was to name the identity of 2- and 3-dimensional shapes. She was shown 20 images of 2-dimensional (2-D: e.g. circle, square) and 3-D geometric shapes (e.g. sphere, cube); see Figure 1c.

The results show that EE555 described all but 1 shape as 2-D. Cubes and rectangular boxes were described as squares or rectangles; cylinders as circles, and pyramids as triangles. The exception was the sphere, which she accurately identified. Overall accuracy was 80% for the 2-D shapes (she called 2 diamonds triangles) and 10% for the 3-D shapes, leading to an overall accuracy level of 45% (9/20). These data suggest that EE555 is impaired at accessing perspective cues.

Experiment 3: Shadow

Circles can appear to be protruding bumps or sunken depressions depending on the position of shadows and shading (Ramachandran, 1988), see Figure 1d. Here, we examined EE555's ability to access shadow and shading cues. EE555 was pseudorandomly presented with 10 protruding and 10 sunken circles. EE555 correctly stated that the protruding circles ‘puffed out’ 100% of the time (10/10 trials) but incorrectly stated that the sunken circle appeared “flat” rather than sunken 100% of the time (10/10 trials). These data suggest that in addition to difficulty perceiving occlusion and linear perspective cues, she has difficulty interpreting shadow and shading cues. The findings of Part 1 show that EE555 has irregular access to several monocular distance cues.

Part 2. Reference Frame and Proximity

In Part 2 we asked whether the patient's performance differed as a function of reference frame: egocentric or allocentric. This comparison was prompted by studies suggesting that dissociable neural systems exist for egocentric and allocentric processing. Specifically, neurons in the monkey lateral intraparietal area – an area thought to be homologous to the human angular gyrus – respond to the egocentric distance between an object and a body part, such as the monkey's hand (Genovesio and Ferraina, 2004; Sakata, Taira, Kusunoki, Murata, and Tanaka, 1997). In contrast, damage to the ventral visual stream extending into the medial temporal lobe, causes disproportionate deficits in allocentric spatial memory (Carey, Dijkerman, and Milner, 1998; Hartley et al., 2007; Holdstock, Mayes, Cezayirli, Aggleton, and Roberts, 1999; Holdstock et al., 2000; Murphy, Carey, and Goodale, 1998; but see Shrager, Bayley, Bontempi, Hopkins, and Squire, 2007). The well-known patient DF, who has bilateral ventral stream lesions, exhibits intact egocentric task performance, but impaired performance when forced to apply an allocentric reference frame (Carey et al., 1998; Carey, Dijkerman, Murphy, Goodale, and Milner, 2006; Murphy et al., 1998). These reports suggest that temporal lobe structures perform computations essential for processing objects in an allocentric reference frame (Murphy et al., 1998). These findings predict that patient EE555 would have a greater impairment processing distance in an egocentric reference frame.

In Part 2 we also examined distance perception as a function of peripersonal space (within arm's reach) and extrapersonal space (beyond reaching distance). In humans, peripersonal and extrapersonal space are thought to be processed by separable neural systems: peripersonal space relies on inferior parietal regions including the supramarginal and post-central gyri, whereas extrapersonal space processing is thought to rely on ventral premotor, middle frontal and superior temporal areas (Committeri et al., 2007). Further evidence comes from the neuropsychology literature reporting patients with hemispatial neglect limited to peripersonal space (Berti and Frassinetti, 2000; Guariglia and Antonucci, 1992), or restricted to extrapersonal space (Bisiach, Perani, Vallar, and Berti, 1986). Given the location of patient EE555's occipitoparietal lesions, these studies predict that she would show greater deficits in peripersonal space.

Experiment 4: Egocentric Distance Judgments in the Z-plane

Methods

Control Subjects

Performance was compared to that of two normal control subjects (both female; 56 and 44 years old; 14 and12 years of education, respectively).

Stimuli

Two types of stimuli were used, (1) an unfamiliar object stimuli, a 5 cm3 white cube and (2) a familiar object stimuli, a 20-ounce plastic bottle.

Procedure

The experimenter placed one object at one of four possible distances: 6, 12, 24, or 36 inches. The task was to verbally estimate the distance of the object. Throughout Part 2, trials with the familiar and unfamiliar objects were randomized, as were the distances and ten measurements for each object at each distance were taken. There were a total of 80 trials.

Analysis

Experiments in Part 2 compared the performance of EE555 with age- and education-matched female control subjects. Because the tasks were trivially easy, control subjects’ performance in many cases showed minimal variability, making statistical comparisons using z-scores impossible. Instead, we report the difference score (DS) between each group estimate and the correct response. We also provide a “spatial index” (SI) that shows judged distances normalized by the correct distance to facilitate comparisons across conditions:

Spatial Index (SI) = (Performance Mean − Actual Distance) ∕ Actual Distance.

With this measure, an SI score of 0 reflects perfect performance, distance underestimations fall below 0, and overestimations above 0. The absolute value of the SI indicates the magnitude of distance judgment errors as a proportion of the actual distance being judged. For the purposes of this paper, we consider SI absolute values greater than .5 to represent a significant difference. This arbitrary cutoff value provides a conservative assessment of function that allows for considerable measurement errors. For example an SI of .5 corresponds to an error of plus or minus 3 inches when the object was placed at 6 inches or an error of 18 inches when the object was placed at 36 inches.

Results and Discussion

The control subjects estimated the distances of familiar and unfamiliar objects with high accuracy and estimation errors were small (all DS < 1.5 inches); see Figure 2 and Table 2. Likewise, patient EE555 accurately estimated the distance of familiar objects (all SI < .1). However, she inaccurately estimated the distance of unfamiliar objects. She overestimated the distance of unfamiliar objects by more than 15.0 inches at every location. EE555's SI scores for the unfamiliar object met our criteria for significance at each distance with greater SI values at the closer distances (SI unfamiliar = 2.6, 2.7, 1.2, 1.1). These results demonstrate that EE555 was particularly inaccurate when making egocentric distance judgments for unfamiliar objects and that her performance was worse at closer distances. The present study required verbal distance estimates. In the following studies we asked subjects to place the object at a given distance. Our logic was that if the patient were permitted to use her sense of proprioception, she might achieve normal performance.

Which of the following help us estimate objects’ size and distance to aid in our perception?

Results of Experiment 4. Left: Estimated distance as a function of actual object placement. Bars indicate object distance estimation in inches. The patient's error bars reflect the within subject variance. The control subjects’ error bars reflect the standard error of the mean. Right: The spatial indices (SI) are plotted as a function of actual distance. Performance outside the two dashed lines is considered significantly abnormal.

Table 2

Summary of performance for patient EE555 and control subjects for Experiments 4−7. For each experiment, the mean distance for each group is provided in inches. In parentheses is EE555's standard deviation, and for the controls, the standard error of the mean is given. These values provide an assessment of within subject variability across tasks.

Familiar ObjectUnfamiliar Object
Exp 4 6 in 12 in 24 in 36 in 6 in 12 in 24 in 36 in
EE555 6.0 12.0 24.0 33.6 21.6 44.4 52.8 76.8
(0) (0) (5.7) (7.6) (5.1) (9.9) (14.1) (24.1)
Control 6.0 12.0 24.0 34.5 6.0 11.75 24.0 34.7
(0) (0) (0) (1.5) (0) (.25) (0) (1.4)
Exp 5
EE555 16.6 20.2 27.8 - 19.6 25.2 32.0 -
(4.2) (5.9) (5.8) (5.7) (4.8) (5.4)
Control 5.8 12.1 22.7 - 5.95 13.3 23.1 -
(1.1) (1.1) (.8) (.9) (1.8) (.07)
Exp 6
EE555 6.6 12.7 18.9 24.8 9.7 17.7 26.6 36.7
(1.6) (2.5) (3.3) (2.7) (9.7) (5.3) (4.6) (2.0)
Control 5.7 13.2 22.4 39.7 6.1 12.8 23.1 40.9
(.6) (.3) (.28) (3.4) (.02) (.01) (.1) (4.1)
Exp 7
EE555 5.2 8.1 17.5 30.2 6.5 13.8 25.7 37.0
(1.8) (2.1) (4.2) (2.5) (1.3) (3.7) (6.1) (3.9)
Control 6.6 13.0 21.4 34.8 7.2 13.1 22.7 36.3
(.6) (1.1) (2.0) (4.8) (.8) (.6) (.6) (3.2)

Experiment 5. Egocentric Object Placement in the Z-Plane

Methods

The same subjects, objects and general procedure were repeated from Experiment 4. The primary change was that subjects placed the object at a verbally instructed distance. The second difference was that the 36-inch condition was eliminated because it was beyond the reach of seated subjects.

Results and Discussion

The control subjects performed with high accuracy; see Figure 3 and Table 2. In contrast, patient EE555 placed the familiar and unfamiliar object beyond the instructed distance across all conditions (DS familiar = 10.6, 8.2, 3.8 inches; DS unfamiliar = 13.6, 13.2, 8.0 inches). The SI scores show that the magnitude of EE555's impairment reached significance at the closer two distances (SI familiar = 1.8, .7; SI unfamiliar = 2.3, 1.1). At the 24-inch distance, her SI score was within the normal range. These findings are consistent with the findings of Experiment 4 in which performance was more impaired at closer distances, but here, her performance was poor for both the familiar and unfamiliar object. These data suggest that the object placement task reduced the benefit of familiar object size, which she could use to estimate distance in Experiment 4. In the next experiment, we tested whether distance estimates were equally impaired when using an allocentric reference frame.

Which of the following help us estimate objects’ size and distance to aid in our perception?

Results of Experiment 5. Left: Object distance placements in an egocentric reference frame. EE555 placed both the familiar and unfamiliar object at greater distances than control subjects. Error bars for EE555 represent the standard deviation of her responses, error bars for the control subjects represent the standard error of the mean. Right: The spatial indices are plotted as a function of actual distance. Performance outside the two dashed lines is considered significantly abnormal.

Experiment 6: Allocentric Object Placement in the Z-plane

Methods

One previously tested and one new control subject participated (both female; 56, 53 years old; 14, 15 years of education). The procedure was modified from the previous experiment in several respects. Four distances were tested (6-, 12-, 24-, 36- inches). Two objects were used during each trial, either two unfamiliar cubes (5 cm3) or two familiar plastic bottles. One item served as a reference object placed approximately 8 inches in front of the subject and remained stationary during the trial. Subjects were verbally instructed to place an object a designated distance behind a reference object thereby employing an allocentric reference frame.

Results and Discussion

Control subjects performed with high accuracy when placing the object using an allocentric reference frame; see Figure 3 and Table 2. Patient EE555 performed accurately when placing the familiar object. For the unfamiliar object, she tended to overestimate the placement of objects at the two closer points (6 and 12 inches; SI = .6, .5) but performed normally when placing objects at the two farther distances (SI < .5). These data indicate that the patient was less impaired when using an allocentric reference frame. However, performance at the closest locations remained less accurate than performance at farther locations. These data suggest that allocentric performance in the depth plane remains generally intact.

Whether these findings should generalize to the frontoparallel, or x-y plane, is unclear. Hemispatial neglect caused by unilateral right parietal damage is frequently associated with deficits in the x-y plane (reviewed in Mesulam, 1999). In contrast there is a report of a patient with bilateral parietal damage performing a reaching task with better performance in the x-y plane as compared to the depth plane (Baylis & Baylis, 2001). For this reason, we investigated allocentric distance performance in the x-y plane.

Experiment 7: Allocentric Object Placement in the X-Y Plane

Methods

The subjects, stimuli and procedure were identical to those used in Experiment 6. The only difference was that subjects were instructed to place the familiar or unfamiliar object to the right of the reference object in the frontoparallel or X-Y plane.

Results and Discussion

All study participants performed with similarly high accuracy across all conditions; see Figure 5 and Table 2. EE555 tended to underestimate distances when using familiar objects, but none of the SI scores reached significance (all SI < .35). These data suggest that the patient can assess distance according to an allocentric reference frame and is less impaired when interacting in the X-Y plane than when performing in the Z plane. These data provide additional support for the view that EE555's performance when using an allocentric reference frame is generally intact.

Which of the following help us estimate objects’ size and distance to aid in our perception?

Experiment 7 results. Left: Allocentric object placement in the frontoparallel plane. Error bars on EE555's bars reflect standard deviation; error bars on the control subject bars represent the standard error of the mean. Right: The spatial indices are plotted as a function of the actual distance of the object. Performance outside the two dashed lines is considered significantly abnormal.

Part 3. Estimation of Size and Distance in the Absence of Stimulus Familiarity

In Part 3 we examined whether EE555 experiences comparable disruptions in distance and size perception. Although dissociations of size and distance perception can occur (Gruber, 1954), such a dissociation would be surprising since size estimates are linked to distance estimates via size constancy. We examined whether EE555 was impaired at estimating the size of novel objects and if so, whether performance relied on binocular (Experiment 8) or monocular (Experiment 9) vision. We predicted that in contrast to her self-reports of normal size perception, she would be impaired at estimating object size.

Experiment 8: Binocular Size and Distance Judgments with Unfamiliar Objects

Methods

EE555's performance was compared to one female age- (41 years), and education- (14 years) matched normal control subject. The experimenter first placed either the 5 cm3 or 10 cm3 cube at one of three distances: 28.5, 57 and 114 cm; see Table 3. Subjects were instructed to close their eyes until permitted to view the first presentation. Subsequently, they closed their eyes while the cube was removed and opened them when the second presentation was ready. There were approximately 2 seconds between presentations. Subjects remained still to prevent them from employing motion parallax to aid their judgments. There were two tasks. In the size judgment task, subjects were asked which cube was larger. In the distance judgment task, subjects were asked which cube was farther. Both tasks were 3-alternative forced-choice (chance = 33%) with the following possible verbal responses: ‘1st cube’, ‘2nd cube’ or ‘same size/distance’.

Table 3

Summary of testing conditions for Experiments 8 and 9.

TESTING CONDITIONS
Constant Physical Size
Cube 1 Distance Visual Cube 2 Distance Visual
(cm3) (cm) Angle ° (cm3) (cm) Angle °
5 28.5 10.0 5 57.0 5.0
5 28.5 10.0 5 114.0 2.5
5 57.0 5.0 5 114.0 2.5
10 57.0 10.0 10 114.0 5.0
Constant Retinal Size
5 28.5 10.0 10 57.0 10.0
5 57.0 5.0 10 114.0 5.0

There were two experimental conditions and a control condition. In the constant physical size condition the same cube was presented at different distances (e.g. the 5 cm3 cube at 57 and 114 cm), so that retinal size differed. In the constant retinal size condition the large cube was presented at double the distance of the small cube (e.g. the 5 cm3 cube at 57 cm and the 10 cm3 cube at 114 cm), so that retinal size remained constant. In the control condition the same cube was presented twice at the same distance. The trial types were randomized. The patient was tested in 2 binocular sessions; the control subject was tested in 1 binocular session. Sessions were separated by at least 2 months. EE555 performed a total of 63 size judgment trials: 18 control, 23 constant physical size and 22 constant retinal size trials. She performed 54 distance judgment trials: 10 control trials, 22 constant physical and 22 constant retinal trials.

Analysis

Because the task was 3-alternative forced choice and there were no distance estimations, we could not use the SI measurement. Instead, performance was evaluated using Fisher's Exact Probability Test (Preacher and Briggs, 2001). We compared patient and control performance and also EE555's performance to chance (33%). This test evaluates 2 × 2 contingency tables describing mutually exclusive outcome counts for two groups, and provides the probability (p) of skewed results as great or greater than that observed. For each comparison, the percentage of correct answers and Fisher p values are reported. Values below .05 were considered significant. Because control performance was at ceiling (100% consistent with size constancy) in both Experiments 8 and 9, we report one-tailed significance values.

Results and Discussion

Control Trials

When the same cube was presented twice at the same distance, EE555 performed no differently from the control subject when making size judgments (94.4%, p = .64, see Figure 6, Table 4) or distance judgments (90.0%, p = .52). These data suggest that EE555 was compliant and able to perform the tasks. These data also indicate that poor performance was not due to memory demands imposed by the experimental paradigm.

Which of the following help us estimate objects’ size and distance to aid in our perception?

Top Left. Experiment 8 (binocular viewing) results for the size and distance tasks when the physical size of the object remains constant, but the retinal size varied. Top Right. Experiment 8 results for the size and distance tasks when the retinal size remained constant, but the actual physical size of the two objects differs. The middle row provides a cartoon depiction of the physical constant and retinal constant conditions. Bottom Left. Experiment 9 (monocular viewing) results for the size and distant tasks when the physical size was constant. Bottom Right. Experiment 9 results for the size and distant tasks when the retinal size is constant.

Table 4

Data from Part 3. Size constancy accuracy for constant retinal size and constant physical size conditions and for both viewing conditions. The headers refer to the three possible answers; in the constant retinal size conditions the answer refers to cube size and in the constant physical condition the answer refers to retinal size – correct answers are indicated by bold headings. Across tasks, the control subject was 100% accurate. Bi = binocular viewing, Mono = monocular viewing.


Size Judgments

Constant Retinal Size (%)
Constant Physical Size (%)
LargerSmallerSameLargerSmallerSame
EE555 Bi 63.6 31.8 4.6 91.3 0 8.7
EE555 Mono 95.0 5.0 0 78.3 17.4 4.3

Distance Judgments

Constant Retinal Size (%)
Constant Physical Size (%)
FartherCloserSameFartherCloserSame
EE555 Bi 27.3 63.6 9.1 54.5 4.5 41.0
EE555 Mono 5.0 90.0 5.0 69.6 8.7 21.7

Size Judgment Task

When retinal size was held constant and physical size differed, EE555 was impaired relative to the control subject (63.6%, p = .05) but her performance was significantly better than chance (p = .03). When physical size was constant and retinal sizes differed, EE555 selected the cube with larger retinal size, leading to extremely poor performance (8.7%, p < .0001) that was significantly below chance (p = .0001). These data indicate that EE555 relies heavily on retinal size cues when making size judgments. These data also suggest that when retinal size was held constant she accessed some unknown cue to make her decision and perform at above-chance accuracy.

Distance Judgment Task

When retinal size or physical size was constant, EE555 was significantly impaired at distance judgments (retinal constant: 27.2%, p = .0005; physical constant: 54.4%, p = .001). Performance was no different from chance for both conditions (p's > .11). These data support the view that she relies on retinal size when making distance judgments; objects with smaller retinal sizes were judged as farther. When retinal size was constant, EE555's distance judgments fell to chance performance.

Between Task Comparison

EE555's performance shows that she was better at making size judgments than distance judgments when retinal size remained constant (p = .03, two-tailed). When physical size was constant the reverse pattern was observed; EE555 was better able to make distance judgments than size judgments (p = .001, two-tailed). This analysis suggests that size judgments were not as reliant on retinal size cues as distance judgments because she performed above chance when retinal size was constant. However, they also show that when there was any difference in retinal size, EE555 chose the smaller retinal size as farther (performing accurately) and as smaller (performing inaccurately).

Experiment 9: Monocular Size and Distance Judgments with Unfamiliar Objects

Methods

EE555's performance was compared to two female age- (41, 38 years), and education- (14, 16 years) matched normal control subjects. The sole change from Experiment 8 is that viewing was monocular. Subjects wore eye patches over their non-dominant (left) eye. The patient was tested in 3 monocular sessions separated by at least 2 months; the control subjects were tested in 1 monocular session. EE555 performed a total of 63 monocular trials: 23 control trials, 23 constant physical size, and 20 constant retinal size. The control subjects performed a total of 49 trials per task.

Results and Discussion

Control Trials

EE555 performed normally in the size judgment (91.3%, p = .74) and distance judgment tasks (89.7%, p = .74). These data confirm that EE555 was able to perform the task under monocular conditions.

Size Judgment Task

When the retinal size was constant, performance was not statistically different from control performance (74.1%, p = .13). When physical size was held constant, EE555 incorrectly chose the object with larger retinal size, leading to impaired performance (3.3%, p = .0000002) that was significantly below chance (p = .003). This pattern is an enhanced version of what was observed under binocular conditions. There was a greater degree of preserved performance when retinal size was held constant. When retinal size differs, EE555 selected the item with greatest retinal size as the largest item.

Distance Judgment Task

When the retinal size was constant, EE555 was impaired (14.8%, p = .00002), and did not differ statistically from chance performance (p = .10). In contrast, when the physical size was constant, EE555's performance was not different from the control group (70.0%, p = .09). These data can also be explained by a retinal size account. EE555 was impaired when retinal size cues were constant, but when they differed she selected the item with smaller retinal size as the more distant item.

Between Task Comparison

When retinal size was held constant, EE555 was better able to make size judgments than distance judgments (p = .00002, two-tailed). When physical size was held constant, she was better able to make distance than size judgments (p = .000008, two-tailed). These results were similar to the results described above and can be explained by a reliance on retinal size cues when the physical size was constant. The most surprising finding was that when retinal size was constant, EE555 was able to make size judgments but not distance judgments. This dissociation in task performance suggests that the mechanisms underlying size and distance judgments are partially dissociable.

General Discussion

Neuroimaging findings indicate that a region of the dorsal visual stream around the junction of the occipital and parietal lobes is consistently activated when processing stereoscopic depth (Backus et al., 2001; Brouwer et al., 2005; Kwee et al., 1999; Neri et al., 2004; Nishida et al., 2001; Quinlan and Culham, 2007; Tsao et al., 2003). Here we investigated whether this region is critically involved in monocular as well as binocular distance perception, and how these functions interact with size perception. Based on a review of the literature, we predicted that a patient with bilateral occipitoparietal lesions would exhibit (1) greater deficits when computing distance in egocentric as compared to allocentric reference frames; (2) greater deficits when computing distance in peripersonal as compared to extrapersonal space; and (3) deficits estimating object size. In general these predictions were supported.

The brain damage suffered by patient EE555 has led to a nearly complete elimination of stereopsis and an inconsistent ability to utilize several common and potent monocular pictorial distance cues: occlusion, perspective, and shadow. When required to verbally estimate the distance between her and an unfamiliar object according to an egocentric reference frame or to place an object at a designated distance from her, EE555 performed abnormally. However, such deficits appear to be largely limited to the egocentric reference frame, as EE555 was less impaired when asked to place objects a given distance from an external reference, either in the depth or horizontal planes. When placing objects in depth, EE555 performed worse at near distances compared to far distances in either reference frame.

Under some conditions, EE555 can accurately estimate distance and size, indicating her ability to access some distance information is preserved. For example, she is adept at using familiar size cues. Such a reliance on familiar size cues has previously been observed when patients with ventral stream damage are forced to rely on monocular distance cues to perform visuomotor tasks (Marotta, Behrmann, and Goodale, 1997; Marotta and Goodale, 2001). It is logical that when the full canon of distance cues is unavailable, a strategy develops that depends on remaining cues such as familiar size. EE555 also makes use of retinal size to estimate size and distance. However, even when familiar size and retinal size cues could not be used, EE555 shows some residual ability to compute object size. This dissociation suggests that the only additional cues that could be reasonably expected to remain present, accommodation and vergence, were accessed when there were no other competing sources of size or distance information.

Accommodation, the process of focusing the lens, and vergence, the process of moving the eyes in opposite directions to focus an object, are often overlooked in discussions of distance processing. Vergence angle can serve as an effective distance cue at distances up to 8 m (McKee and Smallman, 1998; Mon-Williams and Tresilian, 1999). Moreover, vergence angle is known to strongly affect perceived size (Hermans, 1937). Vergence manipulations can in fact generate dissociations of judged size and distance, producing the “size-distance paradox” (Hermans, 1954) in which depth cues lead observers to see closer objects as smaller and consequently judge them as farther (Gruber, 1954). Accommodation can serve as a cue to distance within a restricted range of 2 m. It has been shown that accommodation can modestly affect size perception (Fisher and Ciuffreda, 1988; McLin, Schor, and Kruger, 1988; Wallach and Leggett, 1972) in a manner that optical changes in the size of the retinal image cannot explain (Smith, Meehan, and Day, 1992). Accommodation states are not altered by monocular pictorial cues (Busby and Ciuffreda, 2005; Ittleson and Ames, 1950), although they are yoked to convergence (Martens and Ogle, 1959) so one would not expect the use of accommodation to be impaired by the loss of pictorial cue information.

EE555's normal use of familiar size cues is paralleled by normal mental imagery of common object size. She accurately estimates the length of known objects (e.g. “Show me the length of a pencil”) and will select the larger of two objects (e.g. car vs. truck) from memory (Berryhill, Phuong, Picasso, Cabeza, and Olson, 2007). She also accurately estimates the absolute distance between objects in familiar environments. Patient EE555 commented that she found all of the tasks extremely easy indicating that she is often not cognizant of her perceptual impairment. When asked if she thought the world looked flat, she remarked that it looked the way it always had.

Anatomy of Distance Perception

It is unlikely that the observed distance perception deficits are a consequence of the clinical symptoms of simultanagnosia because these symptoms can be dissociated. Recently a patient with right occipitoparietal damage, lacking simultanagnosia, was reported to have impaired size constancy (Grimsen, Hildebrandt, and Fahle, 2008). Also, we tested a patient with somewhat more superior bilateral parietal lobe lesions and simultanagnosia. This patient has normal stereopsis, distance perception and size perception (patient TQ591, unpublished data).

Neuroimaging studies of stereoscopic distance processing show activation clusters in retinotopic areas in the superior occipital lobe: areas V3A (Backus, Fleet, Parker, and Heeger, 2001; Tsao et al., 2003), V7 (Brouwer et al., 2005; Tsao et al., 2003), and V4dtopo (Brouwer et al., 2005; Tsao et al., 2003). These activations tend to extend superiorally into the caudal intraparietal sulcus (Rutschmann and Greenlee, 2004). The activated regions tend to exhibit greater adaptation to egocentric depth as compared to allocentric depth (Neggers, Van der Lubbe, Ramsey, and Postma, 2006; Neri et al., 2004), suggesting that these areas are especially tuned to the relationship between the viewer and the perceived object. We recently found that a similar region in the superior occipital lobe (V3A) is activated by monocular distance cues (Berryhill, Aguirre, and Olson, 2008). EE555's lesions overlap with the superior occipital activations reported in the reviewed fMRI studies.

EE555's preserved abilities to utilize familiar size information, her probable use of accommodation and vergence, and her ability to compute allocentric distance indicate that these functions rely on neural processing mechanisms that are distinct from those needed to compute distance based on binocular and most monocular pictorial cues. Ventral stream regions intact in EE555 are thought to compute familiar size (e.g. Marotta and Goodale, 2001) and allocentric distance perception (Carey et al., 1998; Hartley et al., 2007; Holdstock et al., 1999; Holdstock et al., 2000; Murphy et al., 1998). One fMRI study has linked a small midline occipitoparietal region to convergence and accommodation (Quinlan and Culham, 2007), a neural region that is also preserved in patient EE555.

Egocentric and Allocentric Reference Frames

There are few neuropsychological examinations of allocentric distance perception. However, several pieces of evidence have linked allocentric spatial memory to the hippocampus. Parietal lobe regions are thought to be essential for egocentric processing (reviewed in Burgess, 2008). The present data are consistent with this view. Patient EE555 exhibited striking egocentric distance judgment impairments but performed relatively normally when using an allocentric reference frame.

EE555's relatively preserved performance when using an allocentric reference frame contrasts with findings from ventral form agnosic patient DF. In this comparison, results from DF and EE555 reveal a double-dissociation. DF is impaired when using an allocentric reference frame but not when using an egocentric reference frame (Carey et al., 2006; Murphy et al., 1998); EE555 is more impaired at using an egocentric reference frame than an allocentric reference frame. These data support the neuroimaging results that indicate independent processing for each of these reference frames (Committeri et al., 2004).

Parietal damage has not always produced deficits in egocentric processing as we observed in EE555. For instance, patients with unilateral parietal lesions and hemispatial neglect may exhibit both allocentric (e.g. line bisection) and egocentric (e.g. deviations of body midline) spatial deficits (reviewed in Kerkhoff, 2001). Findings from patients with bilateral parietal lobe damage is also mixed. A prior study of reaching and distance perception in a patient (RM) with bilateral occipitoparietal damage reported that he was modestly impaired when acting within an egocentric reference frame, more impaired when acting within an allocentric reference frame (Baylis and Baylis, 2001). The source of these mixed findings remains in need of clarification.

Peripersonal and Extrapersonal Space

Parietal patients with spatial neglect may be disproportionately affected in either peripersonal or extrapersonal space (Bisiach et al., 1986). This observation has led to further investigations of the neural correlates of space corroborating the view that dissociable neural networks process peripersonal and extrapersonal space in both humans (Committeri et al., 2007) and monkeys (Previc, 1998). The regions implicated in processing peripersonal space, inferior parietal lobe areas, overlap with patient EE555's lesions. As the present results demonstrate, distance judgments were proportionately worse for the closest distances.

Conclusions

The present data confirm that bilateral lesions to occipitoparietal regions are associated with striking distance and size perception deficits, bearing out the results of neuroimaging studies (Backus et al., 2001; Berryhill et al., 2008; Brouwer et al., 2005; Inui et al., 2000; Iwami et al., 2002; Kwee et al., 1999; Naganuma et al., 2005; Neri et al., 2004; Nishida et al., 2001; Rutschmann and Greenlee, 2004; Rutschmann et al., 2000; Tsao et al., 2003; see review in Parker and Cumming 2001). Importantly, the human brain appears to rely on distinct processing regions for different aspects of distance perception. Both binocular and monocular cues appear to rely on similar processing regions in the dorsal stream, which are specialized for the processing of cues within egocentric and peripersonal frames of reference. In contrast, allocentric and extrapersonal cues appear to rely on computations performed by other cortical areas.

Testing more neuropsychological patients will be required to establish the generality of the current findings, and to map the cortical functional dependencies implied by this investigation. One interesting avenue of further inquiry would be to examine how the computations for egocentric and allocentric distance perception differ. Portions of the dorsal stream may code for or receive input encoding information about one's position in space. If disrupted, the loss of this information may give rise to egocentric spatial deficits. Another interesting question is how egocentric and allocentric maps interact to guide navigation (Whitlock, Sutherland, Witter, Moser, and Moser, 2008). Last, we note that fMRI studies have reported that activity in the parietal lobe is modulated by numerical distance, spatial distance, and temporal distance (Walsh, 2003) thus raising the possibility that distance, broadly defined, may serve as a common metric for dorsal stream processing.

Which of the following help us estimate objects’ size and distance to aid in our perception?

Experiment 6 results. Left: Performance in the allocentric object placement task in the Z, or depth plane. Error bars on EE555's bars reflect the standard deviation of her performance; error bars on the control subject bars reflect the standard error of the mean. Right: Spatial indices (SI) are plotted as a function of the actual distance. Performance outside the two dashed lines is considered significantly abnormal.

Acknowledgements

We thank Marianna Stark, Anjan Chatterjee, Laurel Buxbaum, Melvin Goodale, and Branch Coslett for their advice and assistance. This research was supported by NIH MH071615 to I. Olson and NRSA NS059093 to M.Berryhill.

References

  • Backus BT, Fleet DJ, Parker AJ, Heeger DJ. Human cortical activity correlates with stereoscopic depth perception. J Neurophysiol. 2001;86:2054–2068. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Baylis GC, Baylis LL. Visually misguided reaching in Balint's syndrome. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39:865–875. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Berryhill ME, Aguirre GK, Olson IR. Superior occipital regions track perceived viewing distance in two-dimensional images. J Vision. 2008;8:81a. [Google Scholar]
  • Berryhill ME, Phuong L, Picasso L, Cabeza R, Olson IR. Parietal lobe and Episodic Memory: Bilateral damage causes impaired free recall of autobiographical memory. J Neurosci. 2007;27:14415–14423. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Berti A, Frassinetti F. When far becomes near: remapping of space by tool use. J Cogn Neurosci. 2000;12:415–420. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bisiach E, Perani D, Vallar G, Berti A. Unilateral neglect: personal and extra-personal. Neuropsychologia. 1986;24(6):759–767. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Brouwer GJ, van Ee R, Schwarzbach J. Activation in visual cortex correlates with the awareness of stereoscopic depth. J Neurosci. 2005;25(45):10403–10413. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Burgess N. Spatial cognition and the brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2008;1124:77–97. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Busby A, Ciuffreda KJ. The effect of apparent depth in pictorial images on accommodation. Ophthalmic Physiol Opt. 2005;25(4):320–327. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Carey DP, Dijkerman HC, Milner AD. Perception and action in depth. Conscious Cogn. 1998;7(3):438–453. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Carey DP, Dijkerman HC, Murphy KJ, Goodale MA, Milner AD. Pointing to places and spaces in a patient with visual form agnosia. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(9):1584–1594. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Committeri G, Galati G, Paradis AL, Pizzamiglio L, Berthoz A, LeBihan D. Reference frames for spatial cognition: different brain areas are involved in viewer-, object-, and landmark-centered judgments about object location. J Cogn Neurosci. 2004;16(9):1517–1535. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Committeri G, Pitzalis S, Galati G, Patria F, Pelle G, Sabatini U, et al. Neural bases of personal and extrapersonal neglect in humans. Brain. 2007;130(Pt 2):431–441. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Critchley M. The Parietal Lobes. Edward Arnold; London: 1953. [Google Scholar]
  • Ferber S, Danckert J. Lost in space--the fate of memory representations for non-neglected stimuli. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(2):320–325. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Genovesio A, Ferraina S. Integration of retinal disparity and fixation-distance related signals toward an egocentric coding of distance in the posterior parietal cortex of primates. J Neurophysiol. 2004;91(6):2670–2684. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Grimsen C, Hildebrandt H, Fahle M. Dissociation of egocentric and allocentric coding of space in visual search after right middle cerebral artery stroke. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(3):902–914. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Gruber HE. The relation of perceived size to perceived distance. Am J Psychology. 1954;67(3):411–426. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Guariglia C, Antonucci G. Personal and extrapersonal space: a case of neglect dissociation. Neuropsychologia. 1992;30(11):1001–1009. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hartley T, Bird CM, Chan D, Cipolotti L, Husain M, Vargha-Khadem F, et al. The hippocampus is required for short-term topographical memory in humans. Hippocampus. 2007;17(1):34–48. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Harvey M, Milner AD. Balint's patient. Cognitive Neuropsychology. 1995;12:261–281. [Google Scholar]
  • Hermans TG. Visual size constancy as a function of convergence. J Exp Psychol. 1937;21:307–324. [Google Scholar]
  • Hermans TG. The relationship of convergence and elevation changes to judgments of size. J Exp Psychol. 1954;48:204–208. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Holdstock JS, Mayes AR, Cezayirli E, Aggleton JP, Roberts N. A comparison of egocentric and allocentric spatial memory in medial temporal lobe and Korsakoff amnesics. Cortex. 1999;35(4):479–501. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Holdstock JS, Mayes AR, Cezayirli E, Isaac CL, Aggleton JP, Roberts N. A comparison of egocentric and allocentric spatial memory in a patient with selective hippocampal damage. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38(4):410–425. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Holmes G. Disturbances of visual orientation. Brit J Opthalmology. 1918;2:449–468. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Holmes G, Horrax G. Disturbances of spatial orientaiton and visual attention, with loss of stereoscopic vision. Archives of Neurology and Psychiatry. 1919;1:385–407. [Google Scholar]
  • Inui T, Tanaka S, Okada T, Nishizawa S, Katayama M, Konishi J. Neural substrates for depth perception of the Necker cube; a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in human subjects. Neurosci Lett. 2000;282(3):145–148. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ittleson WH, Ames A. Accommodation, convergence and their relation to apparent distance. J Psychology. 1950;30:43–62. [Google Scholar]
  • Iwami T, Nishida Y, Hayashi O, Kimura M, Sakai M, Kani K, et al. Common neural processing regions for dynamic and static stereopsis in human parieto-occipital cortices. Neurosci Lett. 2002;327(1):29–32. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kerkhoff G. Spatial hemineglect in humans. Prog Neurobiol. 2001;63(1):1–27. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kwee IL, Fujii Y, Matsuzawa H, Nakada T. Perceptual processing of stereopsis in humans: high-field (3.0-tesla) functional MRI study. Neurology. 1999;53(7):1599–1601. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Marotta JJ, Goodale MA. Role of familiar size in the control of grasping. J Cogn Neurosci. 2001;13(1):8–17. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Martens TG, Ogle KN. Observations on accommodative convergence; especially its nonlinear relationships. Am J Ophthalmol. 1959;47(1 Part 2):455–462. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • McKee S, Smallman H. Size and speed constancy. In: Walsh V, Kulikowski J, editors. Perceptual Constancy. Cambridge University Press; Cambridge: 1998. pp. 373–408. [Google Scholar]
  • Mon-Williams M, Tresilian JR. Some recent studies on the extraretinal contribution to distance perception. Perception. 1999;28(2):167–181. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Murphy KJ, Carey DP, Goodale MA. The perception of spatial relations in a patient with visual form agnosia. Cogn Neuropsychology. 1998;15:705–722. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Naganuma T, Nose I, Inoue K, Takemoto A, Katsuyama N, Taira M. Information processing of geometrical features of a surface based on binocular disparity cues: an fMRI study. Neurosci Res. 2005;51(2):147–155. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Neggers SF, Van der Lubbe RH, Ramsey NF, Postma A. Interactions between ego- and allocentric neuronal representations of space. Neuroimage. 2006;31(1):320–331. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Neri P, Bridge H, Heeger DJ. Stereoscopic processing of absolute and relative disparity in human visual cortex. J Neurophysiol. 2004;92(3):1880–1891. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Nishida Y, Hayashi O, Iwami T, Kimura M, Kani K, Ito R, et al. Stereopsis-processing regions in the human parieto-occipital cortex. Neuroreport. 2001;12(10):2259–2263. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Parker AJ, Cumming BG. Cortical mechanisms of binocular stereoscopic vision. Prog Brain Res. 2001;134:205–216. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Preacher KJ, Briggs NE. Calculation for Fisher's Exact Test: An interactive calculation tool for Fisher's exact probability test for 2 × 2 tables. 2001. http://www.quantpsy.org.
  • Previc FH. The neuropsychology of 3-D space. Psychol Bull. 1998;124(2):123–164. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Quinlan DJ, Culham JC. fMRI reveals a preference for near viewing in the human parieto-occipital cortex. Neuroimage. 2007;36:167–187. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ramachandran VS. Perception of shape from shading. Nature. 1988;331(6152):163–166. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Riddoch G. Dissociation of visual perceptions due to occipital injuries, with especial reference to appreciation of movement. Brain. 1917;40:15–57. [Google Scholar]
  • Rode G, Michel C, Rossetti Y, Boisson D, Vallar G. Left size distortion (hyperschematia) after right brain damage. Neurology. 2006;67(10):1801–1808. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rutschmann RM, Greenlee MW. BOLD response in dorsal areas varies with relative disparity level. Neuroreport. 2004;15(4):615–619. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rutschmann RM, Schrauf M, Greenlee MW. Brain activation during dichoptic presentation of optic flow stimuli. Exp Brain Res. 2000;134(4):533–537. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Sakata H, Kusunoki M. Organization of space perception: neural representation of three-dimensional space in the posterior parietal cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 1992;2(2):170–174. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Sakata H, Taira M, Kusunoki M, Murata A, Tanaka Y. The TINS Lecture. The parietal association cortex in depth perception and visual control of hand action. Trends Neurosci. 1997;20(8):350–357. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Shrager Y, Bayley PJ, Bontempi B, Hopkins RO, Squire LR. Spatial memory and the human hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007;104(8):2961–2966. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Steeves JK, Gonzalez EG, Steinbach MJ. Vision with one eye: a review of visual function following unilateral enucleation. Spat Vis. 2008;21(6):509–529. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Tsao DY, Vanduffel W, Sasaki Y, Fize D, Knutsen TA, Mandeville JB, et al. Stereopsis activates V3A and caudal intraparietal areas in macaques and humans. Neuron. 2003;39(3):555–568. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Walsh V. Time: the back-door of perception. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003;7(8):335–338. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Whitlock JR, Sutherland RJ, Witter MP, Moser MB, Moser EI. Navigating from hippocampus to parietal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2008;105(39):14755–14762. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Wyke M. Alterations of size constancy associated with brain lesions in man. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1960;23:253–261. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Which of the following helps us detect the movement of stimuli?

Mechanoreceptors. Mechanoreceptors detect stimuli such as touch, pressure, vibration, and sound from the external and internal environments.

Is a key component of Gestalt approaches to perception?

Top-down processing is a key component in Gestalt approaches to perception.

Which of the following parts of a sensory system is responsible for transforming stimulus energy into neural signals?

Sense receptors are specialized neural cells that change physical energy into neural impulses. 2. Each sense receptor is a transducer, a mechanism that converts energy from one form to another.

What is the study of how physical events relate to psychological perceptions of those events?

Psychophysics is the branch of psychology that studies the effects of physical stimuli on sensory perceptions and mental states.